4/11/2022
88
  • Your test results will be posted to your LabCorp Patient™ portal account two to seven days after the information is reported to your doctor. Depending on the complexity of the tests, results may be sent to your doctor the day after your specimen is submitted for testing, or it could take several days to complete the tests and send the results.
  • Some Republican Senators Walk Back Objections To Election Results: Capitol Insurrection Updates Sen. Kelly Loeffler, R-Ga., said she could no longer object to the results 'in good conscience.

President Donald Trump called on Vice President Mike Pence to send key battleground states’ electoral results back to state legislatures during the Joint Session of Congress on Jan. See maps and real-time presidential election results for the 2020 US election.

Single n-back task with visual stimuli.

Results Backend Celery

The n-back task is a continuous performance task that is commonly used as an assessment in psychology and cognitive neuroscience to measure a part of working memory and working memory capacity.[1] The n-back was introduced by Wayne Kirchner in 1958.[2] Some researchers have argued that n-back training may increase IQ, but evidence is mixed.[citation needed]

The task[edit]

The subject is presented with a sequence of stimuli, and the task consists of indicating when the current stimulus matches the one from n steps earlier in the sequence. The load factor n can be adjusted to make the task more or less difficult.

To clarify, the visual n-back test is similar to the classic memory game of 'Concentration'. However, instead of different items that are in a fixed location on the game board, there is only one item, that appears in different positions on the game board during each turn. '1-N' means that you have to remember the position of the item, one turn back. '2-N' means that you have to remember the position of the item two turns back, and so on.

For example, an auditory three-back test could consist of the experimenter reading the following list of letters to the test subject:

T L H C H O C Q L C K L H C Q T R R K C H R

The subject is supposed to indicate when the letters marked in bold are read, because those correspond to the letters that were read three steps earlier.

The n-back task captures the active part of working memory. When n equals 2 or more, it is not enough to simply keep a representation of recently presented items in mind; the working memory buffer also needs to be updated continuously to keep track of what the current stimulus must be compared to. To accomplish this task, the subject needs to both maintain and manipulate information in working memory.[1]

Dual n-back[edit]

Results Back

The dual-taskn-back task is a variation that was proposed by Susanne Jaeggi et al. in 2003.[3] In the dual-task paradigm, two independent sequences are presented simultaneously, typically using different modalities of stimuli, such as one auditory and one visual.

Several smart phone apps and online implementations of the dual n-back task exist.[4]

Applications[edit]

Assessment[edit]

The n-back task was developed by Wayne Kirchner for his research into short-term memory; he used it to assess age differences in memory tasks of 'rapidly changing information'.[2]

Construct validity[edit]

There is some question about the construct validity of the n-back task. While the task has strong face validity and is now in widespread use as a measure of working memory in clinical and experimental settings, there are few studies which explore the convergent validity of the n-back task with other measures of working memory.[5] Those studies have largely revealed weak or modest correlations between individuals' performance on the n-back task and performance on other standard, accepted assessments of working memory.[5][6]

Results backend celery

There are two main hypotheses for this weak correlation between the n-back task and other working memory assessments. One proposal is that the n-back task assesses different 'sub-components' of working memory than do other assessments. A more critical explanation is that rather than primarily assessing working memory, performance on the n-back task depends on 'familiarity- and recognition-based discrimination processes,' whereas valid assessments of working memory demand 'active recall.'[6] Whatever the cause of the performance differences between the n-back and other assessments of working memory, some researchers stress the need for further exploration of the construct validity of the n-back task.[5]

Performance on the n-back task seems to be more closely correlated with performance on measures of fluid intelligence than it is with performance on other measures of working memory (which is also correlated with performance on measures of fluid intelligence).[6] In the same vein, training on the n-back task appears to improve performance on subsequent fluid intelligence assessments, especially when the training is at a higher n-value.[6]

Treatment[edit]

A 2008 research paper claimed that practicing a dual n-back task can increase fluid intelligence (Gf), as measured in several different standard tests.[7] This finding received some attention from popular media, including an article in Wired.[8] However, a subsequent criticism of the paper's methodology questioned the experiment's validity and took issue with the lack of uniformity in the tests used to evaluate the control and test groups.[9] For example, the progressive nature of Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (APM) test may have been compromised by modifications of time restrictions (i.e., 10 minutes were allowed to complete a normally 45-minute test). The authors of the original paper later addressed this criticism by citing research indicating that scores in timed administrations of the APM are predictive of scores in untimed administrations.[10]

The 2008 study was replicated in 2010 with results indicating that practicing single n-back may be almost equal to dual n-back in increasing the score on tests measuring Gf (fluid intelligence). The single n-back test used was the visual test, leaving out the audio test.[10] In 2011, the same authors showed long-lasting transfer effect in some conditions.[11]

Two studies published in 2012 failed to reproduce the effect of dual n-back training on fluid intelligence. These studies found that the effects of training did not transfer to any other cognitive ability tests.[12][13] In 2014, a meta-analysis of twenty studies showed that n-back training has small but significant effect on Gf and improve it on average for an equivalent of 3-4 points of IQ.[14] In January 2015, this meta-analysis was the subject of a critical review due to small-study effects.[15]

A more recent and extended meta-analysis in January 2017[16] also found that n-back training produces a medium improvement in unrelated n-back training tasks, but a small improvement in unrelated working memory (WM) tasks:

The present meta-analysis on the efficacy of n-back training shows medium transfer effects to untrained versions of the trained n-back tasks and small transfer effects to other WM tasks, cognitive control, and Gf [fluid intelligence]. Our results suggest that previous meta-analyses investigating the effects of WM training have overestimated the transfer effects to WM by including untrained variants of the training tasks in their WM transfer domain. Consequently, transfer of n-back training is more task-specific than has previously been suggested.

The question of whether n-back training produces real-world improvements to working memory remains controversial.[17]

Use in tutoring and rehabilitation[edit]

The n-back is now in use outside experimental, clinical, and medical settings. Tutoring companies utilize versions of the task (in conjunction with other cognitive tasks) to allegedly improve the fluid intelligence of their clients.[18] Tutoring companies and psychologists also utilize the task to improve the focus of individuals with ADHD[18] and to rehabilitate sufferers of traumatic brain injury;[19] experiments have found evidence that practice with the task helps these individuals focus for up to eight months following training.[19] However, much debate remains about whether training on the n-back and similar tasks can improve performance in the long run or whether the effects of training are transient,[18][19] and if the effects of training n-back generalize to general cognitive processing, for instance, to fluid intelligence.[20] Despite the claims of commercial providers, there are some researchers who question whether the results of memory training are transferable. Researchers from the University of Oslo published results of the meta-analytical review analyzing various studies on memory training techniques (including n-back) and concluded that 'training programs give only near-transfer effects, and there is no convincing evidence that even such near-transfer effects are durable.'[21]

Neurobiology of n-back task[edit]

Meta-analysis of 24 n-back neuroimaging studies have shown that during this task the following brain regions are consistently activated: lateral premotor cortex; dorsal cingulate and medial premotor cortex; dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; frontal poles; and medial and lateral posterior parietal cortex.[22]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ abGazzaniga, Michael S.; Ivry, Richard B.; Mangun, George R. (2009). Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind (2nd ed.).
  2. ^ abKirchner, W. K. (1958). 'Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information'. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 55 (4): 352–358. doi:10.1037/h0043688. PMID13539317.
  3. ^Jaeggi, S. M., Seewer, R., Nirkko, A. C., Eckstein, D., Schroth, G., Groner, R., et al., (2003). Does excessive memory load attenuate activation in the prefrontal cortex? Load-dependent processing in single and dual tasks: functional magnetic resonance imaging study, Neuroimage 19(2) 210-225.
  4. ^Roizen, Michael; Oz, Mehmet (2018-01-12). 'Playing brain games may help sharpen your skills'. Houston Chronicle. Hearst. Retrieved 10 November 2018.
  5. ^ abcKane, M.J., Conway, A.R.A, Miura, T.K., & Colflesh, G.J.H (2007). 'Working memory, attention control, and the N-back task: a question of construct validity'(PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 33 (3): 615–622. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.615. PMID17470009.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  6. ^ abcdJaeggi, S.M., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W.J., & Meier, B. (2010). 'The concurrent validity of the N-back task as a working memory measure'. Memory. 18 (4): 394–412. doi:10.1080/09658211003702171. PMID20408039. S2CID42767249.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., Perrig, W. J. (2008),Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 105 no. 19
  8. ^Alexis Madrigal, Forget Brain Age: Researchers Develop Software That Makes You Smarter, Wired, April 2008
  9. ^Moody, D. E. (2009). 'Can intelligence be increased by training on a task of working memory?'. Intelligence. 37 (4): 327–328. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.04.005.
  10. ^ abJaeggi, Susanne M.; Studer-Luethi, Barbara; Buschkuehl, Martin; Su, Yi-Fen; Jonides, John; Perrig, Walter J. (2010). 'The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning -- implications for training and transfer'. Intelligence. 38 (6): 625–635. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.09.001. ISSN0160-2896.
  11. ^Jaeggi, Susanne; et al. (2011). 'Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training'. PNAS. 108 (25): 10081–10086. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10810081J. doi:10.1073/pnas.1103228108. PMC3121868. PMID21670271.
  12. ^Redick, T. S.; Shipstead, Z.; Harrison, T. L.; Hicks, K. L.; Fried, D. E.; Hambrick, D. Z.; Kane, M. J.; Engle, R. W. (2012). 'No Evidence of Intelligence Improvement After Working Memory Training: A Randomized, Placebo-Controlled Study'. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 142 (2): 359–379. doi:10.1037/a0029082. PMID22708717. S2CID15117431.
  13. ^Chooi, W. T.; Thompson, L. A. (2012). 'Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults'. Intelligence. 40 (6): 531–542. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.004.
  14. ^Au, Jacky; et al. (2014). 'Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis'(PDF). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 22 (2): 366–377. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x. PMID25102926.
  15. ^Bogg, Tim; Lasecki, Leanne (22 January 2015). 'Reliable gains? Evidence for substantially underpowered designs in studies of working memory training transfer to fluid intelligence'. Frontiers in Psychology. 5: 1589. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01589. PMC4010796. PMID25657629.
  16. ^Soveri, Anna; Antfolk, Jan; Karlsson, Linda; Salo, Benny; Laine, Matti (1 August 2017). 'Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies'. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 24 (4): 1077–1096. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-1217-0. ISSN1531-5320. Retrieved 6 September 2020.
  17. ^Soveri, Anna; Antfolk, Jan; Karlsson, Linda; Salo, Benny; Laine, Matti (1 August 2017). 'Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies'. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 24 (4): 1077–1096. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-1217-0. ISSN1531-5320. Retrieved 6 September 2020.
  18. ^ abcHurley, Dan (2012-10-31). 'The Brain Trainers'. The New York Times. Retrieved 9 November 2012.
  19. ^ abcHurley, Dan (2012-04-18). 'Can You Make Yourself Smarter?'. The New York Times. Retrieved 9 November 2012.
  20. ^Daniel Willingham (2012-06-19). 'New study: Fluid intelligence not trainable'. Retrieved 2013-04-22.
  21. ^Monica Melby-Lervåg & Charles Hulme (2013). 'Is Working Memory Training Effective? A Meta-Analytic Review'(PDF). Developmental Psychology. 49 (2): 270–291. doi:10.1037/a0028228. PMID22612437.
  22. ^Owen, Adrian M.; McMillan, Kathryn M.; Laird, Angela R.; Bullmore, Ed (2005). 'N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies'. Human Brain Mapping. 25 (1): 46–59. doi:10.1002/hbm.20131. PMC6871745. PMID15846822.

External links[edit]

  • Brain Workshop, an open-source, free, standalone, multi-platform implementation of the n-back task
  • IQ boost, a dual n-back application for iOS
  • Brain N-Back, an n-back task implemented as an Android app
  • Dual-N-Back.io, Dual N-back task, implemented as an open source progressive web application. Can be played in a browser, or cached for offline use on mobile.
  • Dual N-Back Meta-analysis showing medium effect of transfer training to fluid intelligence
Retrieved from 'https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=N-back&oldid=995605942'

Q4 2020
Consolidated Revenue: €45.3m
Organic Growth: + 13.0%
EBITDA margin: 16.8%


ANALYSIS
The revenue for the fourth quarter of 2020 amounts to €45.3m and does not include the French business activities that were sold at the end of the 3rd quarter.
The EBITDA margin is 16.8% and organic growth stands at +13.0%.
For 2020, the revenue amounts to €212.5m, with an EBITDA margin of 13.9%.
This revenue only includes the first three quarters of the French business activities that were sold at the end of September.
Organic growth for 2020, without the French business activities, amounts to 9.2%.
The net cash position, former standards, excluding all the IFRS 16 adjustments, amounts to €105.8m, after the advance on dividends paid during the second semester.

2021 BUDGET
The 2021 budget provides for organic growth at 13% and an EBITDA margin of 13% using as a starting point the 2020 revenue restated for the French activities sold, in other words €169.2 m.

Results Backlash 2020

2021-2024 BUSINESS PLAN
The current perimeter is ideal for establishing precise detailed forecasts over a four-year period.
Hence our posting online today on our Website of a document summarising the main elements concerning:
- The Group’s current status,
- The strategy over the next four years,
- The 2021 budget,
- The 2021-2024 business plan,
- The elements likely to improve our future performance.

How Long Does It Take To Get Mri Results Back


The definition of the alternative performance indicators is to be found on page 15 of the 2019 financial report.


About Sword Group
Sword has 1,900+ IT/Digital & Software specialists present over 5 continents to accompany you in the growth of your organisation in the digital age.
As a leader in technological and digital transformation, Sword has a solid reputation in software publishing and in complex IT & business project management.
Sword optimises your processes and enhances your data.
Agenda
11/03/21
FY2020 annual results presentation meeting
(organisation to be defined)
26/04/21
2021 First Quarter Revenue
Sword Group - 2, rue d’Arlon L-8399 Windhof - investorrelations@sword-group.lu

How Long Does It Take To Get Std Results Back


Results Back From Dominican Republic Deaths

Attachment

Results
obstolemreaconsi.netlify.com – 2021